How to Easily Improve Your Memory

10 surprising and mostly easy ways to improve your memory.

10 surprising and mostly easy ways to improve your memory.

Many of the methods for improving memory–like exercise, chunking, building associations or brain training–involve a fair amount of mental effort.

So here are ten (mostly) very easy ways to improve your memory that are supported by research.

With two or three exceptions, most people can do these with very little effort or expense.

1. Clench your right fist

If you squeeze your right hand into a fist during learning, it can aid memory.

Later on, to aid recall, squeeze your left hand into a fist.

In study by Propper et al. (2013), participants who squeezed their right fist during learning and their left during recall, did better than control groups clenching the other fist or not clenching at all.

2. Chew gum

Chewing gum can help you stay focused on a task and so improve your memory.

A study by Morgan et al. (2013) tested the audio memory of those chewing gum, compared with those who didn’t.

The gum chewers had improved short-term memory compared with non-chewers.

3. Go to sleep

One of the many benefits of sleep is that it makes memory stronger.

That’s because the brain is surprisingly busy during sleep and one of the things it’s doing is working on our memories.

Not only does sleep make our memories stronger, it also restructures and reorganises them.

Studies have shown, for example, that people are more likely to dream about things with a higher value to them, and are subsequently more likely to recall those things (Oudiette et al., 2013).

And, if what’s important to you is learning to play the piano, you could even try listening to the piece while you nap, as one study has shown this helps cement the memory (Anthony et al., 2012).

More on how the mind learns during sleep.

4. Go for a walk

Many people suffer memory problems with advancing years.

But, walking just six miles a week helps to preserve memory in old age.

One study has found that older people who walked six to nine miles per week had greater gray matter volume nine years later than those who were more sedentary (Erickson et al., 2010).

5. Stop smoking

Although the physical benefits of quitting smoking are well-known, it’s less well-known that it will also benefit memory.

That’s because smoking damages the memory, and quitting can almost restore it to normal function (Hefferman et al., 2011).

That’s one more reason to quit (or to be happy that you don’t smoke).

6. Ignore stereotypes

If you think your memory will get much worse with age, then it probably will. It’s a self-fulfilling prophecy.

Older people who are reminded of stereotypes about age and memory perform worse in tests (Hess et al., 2003).

So, suffer fewer memory problems with age by paying no heed to the stereotypes.

7. Read Facebook posts

One study has found that people’s memories are much stronger for posts on Facebook than for sentences from books, or even people’s faces.

Mickes et al. (2013) found that Facebook posts were probably easier to remember because they were ‘mind-ready’: they were already in an easily digestible format and written in spontaneous natural speech.

Facebook is also full of juicy gossip, which probably doesn’t hurt!

8. Sniff rosemary

The smell of the essential oil, rosemary, has been shown to improve long-term memory, mental arithmetic and prospective memory–remembering to do things at certain times.

In one study, participants who sat in a room infused with the scent of rosemary performed better on a memory task than a control group (McCready & Moss, 2013).

9. Lose weight

Like smoking, putting on weight is associated with memory problems–but these are also reversible.

Lose some of the weight and memory function is likely to return.

Petterson et al. (2013) found that older, overweight, women whose weight dropped from an average of 85kg (188 pounds) to 77kg (171 pounds), over six months, saw improved memory function.

10. Turn off the computer and sit quietly

Now that you’ve read this article, it’s time to turn off the computer, tablet or phone and sit quietly.

That’s because when we are idle, the brain is actually still performing important memory functions.

Professor Erik Fransén explains:

“The brain is made to go into a less active state, which we might think is wasteful; but [is] probably [when] memory consolidation […] takes place […].

“When we max out our active states with technology […] we remove from the brain part of the processing, and it can’t work.”

→ Continue reading: Memory and Recall: 10 Amazing Facts You Should Know

Image credit: Davey Van Lienden

Exercise Can Improve Long-Term Memory

The right combination of exercise and learning can improve long-term memory.

The right combination of exercise and learning can improve long-term memory.

It’s now well-established that exercise is not just good for your body; it’s also good for your mind.

Exercise improves mood, information processing and makes your thinking more flexible.

But what effect does exercise have on memory? Well, it depends on the type of memory, since it is split into two main types: short and long.

Working memory

Something like short-term memory is usually called ‘working memory’ by psychologists.

Working memory includes what’s in your conscious mind right now and whatever you’re doing with this information. This might include the gist of the last sentence or two and the meanings, conclusions and thoughts you’re having about them. Roughly speaking, it’s the stuff that your mind is working on right now.

Studies suggest that exercise is broadly beneficial for working memory. After 30 minutes exercise, people’s working memory improves. There’s some evidence that accuracy drops a bit, but this is more than made up for by increases in speed (McMorris et al., 2011).

Long-term memory

For psychologists, long-term memory is pretty much what you think it is: it’s anything that leaves conscious thought to be recalled much later.

The effects of exercise on long-term memory are less clear. Some studies show benefits, others not.

But a recent study suggests that exercise only benefits long-term memory under certain conditions (Schmidt-Kassow et al., 2013).

This study had young women trying to learn new words, either after or while they rode an exercise bike.

What they found was that there was a boost to long-term memory when the new vocab was learned during exercise, but not if the learning took place afterwards.

Studies suggest that the exercise should be relatively low-intensity or the brain gets over-stimulated and the benefits may be lost.

We don’t yet know if these benefits extend to other types of learning, or during other types of exercise, but it’s likely they will.

So, if you’re trying to learn a new language, it might be worth listening to those language tapes while you’re working out. Not only will you be keeping fit, but you’ll also be giving yourself a better chance of laying down stronger long-term memories.

→ Continue reading: Memory and Recall: 10 Amazing Facts You Should Know

Image credit: E’Lisa Campbell

How Memory Works: 21 Psychological Insights

Find out how memory twists, pops, distorts, persists and decays, along with the odd tip on how to improve it.

Find out how memory twists, pops, distorts, persists and decays, along with the odd tip on how to improve it.

“You think you have a memory; but it has you!” —John Irving.

Irving’s quote nicely captures our hunch that we are slaves to our memories. Will I recall someone’s name? What moment from my past will come back to delight, perplex or daunt me? And at other times we ask ourselves why we seem unable to forget.

These uncertainties prompt many people to say their memory is awful, a comment distinguished memory researcher Professor Alan Baddeley hears all the time. But be fair, he argues:

“I have a good memory and would argue, despite its occasionally embarrassing fallibility, that both my memory and yours exceed that of the best computer in terms of capacity, flexibility, and durability.” (Baddeley, 1999)

At times it may not feel like it and that’s partly because human memory follows its own rules, not the ones we imagine or prefer. To help us use our memories more effectively we need a better understanding of how it really works so that, hopefully, we can forgive its eccentricities.

Here are 21 of my favourite articles on the mysteries of memory, culled from PsyBlog’s archives:

  1. How Memory Works: 10 Things Most People Get Wrong – “If we remembered everything we should on most occasions be as ill off as if we remembered nothing.” —William James
  2. Why People’s Names Are So Hard to Remember – Why people’s names are more difficult to remember than their jobs, hobbies or home towns.
  3. How Memories are Distorted and Invented: Misattribution – One evening in 1975 an unsuspecting Australian psychologist, Donald M. Thomson, walked into a television studio to discuss the psychology of eyewitness testimony, little did he know…
  4. Memory Improved 20% by Nature Walk – Short-term memory is improved 20% by walking in nature, or even just by looking at an image of a natural scene.
  5. Absent-Mindedness: A Blessing in Disguise? – The benefits of forgetting.
  6. Mind Pops: Memories That Come From Nowhere  – Cheese grater. Why do odd images suddenly pop into your head for no reason?
  7. How Quickly We Forget: The Transience of Memory – How recall fades over time.
  8. Reconstructing the Past: How Recalling Memories Alters Them – Experiment shows both the enhancing and distorting effects of recall on the original memories.
  9. Can Doodling Improve Memory and Concentration? – Doodling may be more than just a pleasant waste of time and paper.
  10. On the Tip-of-the-Tongue: Blocked Memories – College students have one or two ‘tip-of-the-tongue’ moments a week, while older adults have between two to four per week.
  11. Six Memory Myths – Can flashbulb memories be distorted? Some of the most widespread beliefs about memory are plain wrong.
  12. Memory Improved By Saying Words Aloud – Memory can be improved by vocalising or sub-vocalising words.
  13. Infant Memory Works From Very Early – Some argue it’s impossible for us to remember anything much from before around two to four years of age—but is that true?
  14. Memories Are Made of This – Study records the activation of human brain cells deep inside the living brain as memories are formed and recalled.
  15. Memory Enhanced by a Simple Break After Reading – If you find it difficult to remember what you’ve read, try giving the memory time to consolidate.
  16. The Persistence of Memory – Being unable to forget is a double-edged sword.
  17. 7 Simple Ways to Improve Your Memory Without Any Training – Boost your memory by writing about your problems, predicting your performance and more…
  18. How the Consistency Bias Warps Our Personal and Political Memories – What were your political views a decade ago? How good was your relationship last year? Studies show we often assume things haven’t changed, when in fact they have.
  19. The Temporal Doppler Effect: Why The Future Feels Closer Than The Past – Like the sound of a passing ambulance siren, our perception of time distorts as it shoots by.
  20. Implanting False Memories: Lost in the Mall & Paul Ingram – “Do you swear to tell the truth, the whole truth, or whatever it is that you think you remember?” — Elizabeth Loftus
  21. Offline Learning: How The Mind Learns During Sleep – A nap as short as 6 minutes after learning can help to consolidate learning and improve performance.
  22. 10 Surprising and (Mostly) Easy Ways to Improve Your Memory – Improve your memory by clenching your right fist, chewing gum, walking, ignoring stereotypes, sniffing rosemary and more…

Image credit: Mike Bailey-Gates

Reconstructing the Past: How Recalling Memories Alters Them

The first experiment to show the enhancing and distorting effect of recall.

The first experiment to show the enhancing and distorting effect of recall.

Recently the neurologist and author Oliver Sacks recalled a vivid childhood memory, recounted in his autobiography, Uncle Tungsten.

During WWII he lived in London during the Blitz, and on one occasion:

“…an incendiary bomb, a thermite bomb, fell behind our house and burned with a terrible, white-hot heat. My father had a stirrup pump, and my brothers carried pails of water to him, but water seemed useless against this infernal fire—indeed, made it burn even more furiously. There was a vicious hissing and sputtering when the water hit the white-hot metal, and meanwhile the bomb was melting its own casing and throwing blobs and jets of molten metal in all directions.”

Except when his autobiography came out, one of his older brothers told him he’d misremembered the event. In fact both of them had been at school when the bomb struck so they could not have witnessed the explosion.

The ‘false’ memory, it turned out, was implanted by a letter. Their elder brother had written to them, describing the frightening event, and this had lodged in his mind. Over the years the letter had gone from a third-person report to a first-person ‘memory’.

Turning the memory over in his mind, Sacks writes that he still cannot see how the memory of the bomb exploding can be false. There is no difference between this memory and others he knows to be true; it felt like he was really there.

***

This sort of experience is probably much more common than we might like to imagine. Many memories which have the scent of authenticity may turn out to be misremembered, if not totally fictitious events, if only we could check. Without some other source with which to corroborate, it is hard verify the facts, especially for events that took place long ago.

That these sorts of distortions to memory happen is unquestioned, what fascinates is how it comes about. Does the long passage of time warp the memory, or is there some more active process that causes the change?

A study published recently sheds some light on this process and provides a model for how memories like Sack’s become distorted.

In the experiment participants took part in a self-guided museum tour where they were told to stop at particular exhibits along the way (St. Jacques & Schacter, 2013). These stops on the museum tour are the experimental stand-ins for the events you’ve experienced across your life.

Participants were asked to look at a series of photos showing the stops they visited during their tour, a sort of ‘event-movie’, thus retrieving or reactivating memory. Following retrieval, they were shown a new photo taken at the museum that showed stops that were not part of their tour.

This is something like what goes on when you sit looking out of the window on a wet Sunday afternoon, thinking back to childhood memories. Images come back to you in no particular order: some of which may be true memories, others things you’ve been told or simply fabricated. They are jumbled; the sources unclear and the meanings opaque.

In the study participants returned for a third session and were shown photos and asked whether they’d stopped at the exhibit or not. Once again, some of the time they were shown pairs of exhibits they had or hadn’t looked at and other times real and false memories were mixed up together.

Across the three sessions, then, the researchers had simulated the recall of the jumble of real and false memories that are likely to be returned to consciousness when we try to recall past events. Real aspects of a memory get mixed up with false aspects and the whole confection gets stirred up each time we recall it.

In the study they found that participants’ memories were both enhanced and distorted by the process of recall. People found it easier to remember those exhibits which they were subsequently shown photographs of. This shows that merely recalling a memory is enough to strengthen it.

This is one aspect of the fact that memory is an active, reconstructive process; recalling something is not a neutral act, it strengthens that memory in comparison to the others.

But the study also demonstrated that false memories were also strengthened. In other words when participants falsely recalled seeing a particular exhibit in the second session, this made it more likely to be flagged up as a ‘real’ memory in the third session.

What this is showing is how false memories can grow in the mind. Of course, in real life things don’t happen as cleanly as they do in the psych lab. Our memories and fantasies are intertwined, crossing over and interfering with one another. Thinking about the past continues this process of interweaving.

Something like this may have happened to Sacks. Indeed, while he didn’t actually witness the bomb blast described above, he had been there at an earlier incident, describing it thus:

“One night, a thousand-pound bomb fell into the garden next to ours, but fortunately it failed to explode. All of us, the entire street, it seemed, crept away that night (my family to a cousin’s flat)—many of us in our pajamas—walking as softly as we could (might vibration set the thing off?). The streets were pitch dark, for the blackout was in force, and we all carried electric torches dimmed with red crêpe paper. We had no idea if our houses would still be standing in the morning.”

His brother confirmed that he had been there that time.

***

Anyone who thinks that the main aim of memory is fidelity will find this study disheartening. It seems to underline the fragile nature of memory and the human mind in general.

But there is a way to look at it that is not only more positive, but also hints at ways to make valuable personal changes.

What emerges is that autobiographical memory is not just about accurately remembering what happened when, it is an active construction of the self. How you think about the past helps generate your experience in the moment as well as the decisions you take in the future.

If that autobiographical memory can change based on how you recall the past, then our pasts are not gone and forgotten, they are living parts of who we are. Choosing to recall certain events rather than others is a way of choosing how we live now and what decisions we make in the future.

What’s done may be done, but it’s still open to reinterpretation.

Image credit: Joel-Evelyn-Francois

Memory Enhanced by a Simple Break After Reading

If you find it difficult to remember what you’ve read, try this easy technique.

If you find it difficult to remember what you’ve read, try giving the memory time to consolidate.

I have a great memory for books I’ve read on trains.

I always thought this had something to do with the nature of train travel: the rocking of the carriage, the rhythm of the stops, the continually changing picture window. Perhaps the combination of all these helps induce a focus which is harder to achieve in familiar circumstances.

Or perhaps the answer is simpler.

Psychologists have found that brief resting periods after learning aids memory. In studies, when people take a little rest after learning, say, a string of numbers, they do better in recall than other people who’ve been given another task straight away.

It is thought that this little rest helps consolidate the memory, making it easier to retrieve. On the other hand if you go straight on to another task, the memory doesn’t have a chance to solidify.

I began to wonder if this suggested why I find it easier to recall books I’ve read on trains.

Unlike at home where I read continuously, on a train I tend to stop more frequently to look out of the window or see who is getting on at the next stop. These would be exactly the type of restful periods described in this research.

The problem is these findings have only been shown over very short periods. That is until now.

10-minute break

In a new study the effect of a 10-minute break was tested on participants’ recall of a story 7 days later (Dewar et al., 2012).

They found that even after 7 days people’s memory was enhanced when they took a 10-minute break after reading the story. In fact, 7 days later people who’d taken a break were as good as those trying to recall the story just 15-30 minutes later, but without the break.

So perhaps this helps explain why I have a clearer memory of books I’ve read on trains.

It also shows that one of the pleasures of reading—pausing to let it wash over you—is not only agreeable but also helps you remember what you’ve read.

Image credit: Jens Schott Knudsen

Memory Improved By Saying Words Aloud

New study finds memory improved by vocalising or sub-vocalising words.

New study finds memory improved by vocalising or sub-vocalising words.

Committing words to memory is a notoriously hit-and-miss business. Over the last forty years psychologists have found three methods which consistently improve memory for words:

  1. Imagery: recall is aided by creating an image of what you want to remember.
  2. Elaboration: thinking of associations helps anchor words in your mind.
  3. Generation: memory is improved when you have to put some work in to generate the target. E.g. guess the name of your favourite blog from this cryptic clue: _sy_log.

In research on trying to remember lists of words, these three methods have each produced memory improvements of 10% over simply reading words once.

That might not sound much, but it is an average over many studies and often for things that are hard to remember. Psychologists like testing people with non-words like ‘trackle’ or ‘nosting’ that could be words, but aren’t.

Speak it…

Now, in a new series of studies published in the Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, there’s solid evidence for a fourth which could join the other big three memory enhancers (MacLeod et al., 2010).

And, you’ll be happy to hear, it’s very, very simple. It only involves saying the word you want to remember out loud to yourself. It doesn’t even seem to matter if you don’t vocalise the word, it only has to be mouthed. That’s it.

According to MacLeod et al., saying a word out loud, or at least mouthing it, improves memory by increasing its distinctiveness, i.e. making it unusual compared to others.

Across 8 experiments in which participants were asked to read and remember lists of both words and nonwords, the researchers found memory improvements sometimes greater than 10%. They also ruled out some alternative explanations, finding that improvements were not:

  • At the expense of unmouthed words. The effect was all benefit for the mouthed words and didn’t decrease performance on unmouthed words.
  • A result of “lazy reading” of words read silently.

…but be selective

Of course just reading all the words out loud would destroy the effect because then there’s nothing for words said out loud to be distinctive in comparison with. It’s only going to work when some words are said out loud compared with others not.

So if you’re revising, or reading a report or a book and want to retain more of the important points, the key is to identify the right words and vocalise or sub-vocalise them.

This finding ties in with the general idea that we tend to remember people or things that stand out from the crowd. One gentle reminder though: if you are spotted mouthing random words in public, it’s you that will stand out from the crowd.

→ Continue reading: Memory and Recall: 10 Amazing Facts You Should Know

Image credit: Florian Seroussi

Memory Improved 20% by Nature Walk

New study finds that short-term memory is improved 20% by walking in nature, or even just by looking at an image of a natural scene.

New study finds that short-term memory is improved 20% by walking in nature, or even just by looking at an image of a natural scene.

I’m sitting in front of the computer and I’ve been working too hard for too long without a break. My brain feels like it’s filling with wet cardboard. In fact what I’m doing isn’t writing any more, it’s just typing. I go to the kitchen, stand there for a moment, can’t remember what I’ve come in for, feel foolish, then eat a biscuit.

It doesn’t help.

Continue reading “Memory Improved 20% by Nature Walk”

False Memories Can Influence Behaviour

Participants avoided egg salad sandwiches after false memories of sickness were implanted.

Even when human memory is working normally, it is still frequently unfaithful. Instead of the total recall of, say, a video camera we get something more like a symbolist, or even abstract painting. Sights, sounds and smells are refracted by our minds into memories that often tell more about us than the original events they apparently record.

Continue reading “False Memories Can Influence Behaviour”

Memories Are Made of This

New memory study records the activation of human brain cells deep inside the living brain.

Neurons

New memory study records the activation of human brain cells deep inside the living brain.

The mystery of what happens in our brains when we remember something is fascinating not only from a scientific perspective but also because the experience of recall can be so, well, memorable. Thinking backwards we become sensory time travellers; recalling sights, sounds, events, emotions – all in the blink of an eye. But what happens in our brains when we travel backwards?

Continue reading “Memories Are Made of This”

See How Easily You Can Avoid The Memory Bias

To make decisions that will make us happy in the future we need to acknowledge how the memory bias can warp our predictions.

To make decisions that will make us happy in the future we need to acknowledge how the memory bias can warp our predictions.

Today, making my plans for the upcoming public holiday sent my mind straight back four weeks. Then, heading out into the unseasonably warm spring weather, I had high hopes for a relaxing day off.

Unfortunately everyone else in London had exactly the same idea. The whole day my every plan for having fun was foiled: restaurants were booked up, bars were full to overflowing and when I finally did get sat down at a riverside restaurant, it started raining.

Continue reading “See How Easily You Can Avoid The Memory Bias”

Get free email updates

Join the free PsyBlog mailing list. No spam, ever.