Many Memories Are Never Forgotten — They Just Become Inaccessible (M)

All our memories are still locked away in our minds, but only some of them are accessible.

All our memories are still locked away in our minds, but only some of them are accessible.


Keep reading with a membership

• Adverts removed
• Cancel at any time
• 14 day money-back guarantee


Members can sign in below:

Infant Memory: What Babies And Toddlers Can Remember

How psychologists have examined the emergence of infant memory in our first few years of life.

How psychologists have examined the emergence of infant memory in our first few years of life.

Some argue it’s impossible for us to remember anything much from before around two to four years of age.

Others think our infant memory can go way back – perhaps even to before birth.

The question of ‘infantile amnesia’ is thorny because it’s hard to test whether adults’ earliest memories are real or imagined.

What psychologists have done, though, is examine the emergence of infant memory in our first few years with a series of now classic experiments.

If infant memories really can be laid down early in life then it is certainly possible in theory for adults to remember very early experiences.

Getting a kick out of kicking

One classic experiment, devised by Professor Carolyn Rovee-Collier (now at Rutgers University) and colleagues in the 1960s, provides us an insight into what infants can remember.

This method has produced some great evidence about how and when infants’ memories develops.

In their experiment, Rovee and Rovee (1969) had infants of between 9 and 12 weeks lay comfortably in their cribs at home looking up at a mobile covered with brightly coloured wooden figures.

A cord was then attached to their foot connecting it to the mobile.

This meant that if the infants kicked out the mobile would move.

And, if they kicked out hard, the wooden figures would bump into each other and make a pleasant knocking sound.

The more the little kids kick, the more of a kick they get from the mobile.

If you’re starting to get a whiff of Pavlov and his salivating dog then you’re on the right lines.

This experiment is all about seeing if an infant can be conditioned to kick their foot to make the mobile move.

Researchers first measure infants’ baseline levels of kicking (with mobile unattached), then compare this to kicking that produces an exciting response (wow, the mobile is moving!).

What thy found was that even infants as young as 8 weeks old could learn the association between kicking and the mobile movement.

This learning was still evident over a 45-55 minutes period.

Infant memory

While this initial finding is fairly modest, the use of this procedure has led to all sorts of new findings about infant memory.

For example, subsequent infant memory studies have later substituted a different mobile for the original to see if the infants can spot the difference, thereby testing whether or not they really remember.

In one experiment infants only 8-weeks-old were trained with the mobile over a period of 3 days for 9 minutes each day.

Twenty-four hours later the infants only kicked at above their baseline levels when the same mobile was above their heads.

This showed they remembered the particular mobile they had been trained with and not just any old mobile.

It was an especially exciting finding because it had previously been thought that long-term memory (and 24 hours is long-term for psychologists) didn’t emerge until as late as 8 or 9 months.

Because of this experiment and others like it, we now know much more about infant memory.

Our memory systems actually work quite well from very early on.

Infants’ memories also seems to work in much the same way as adult memories – it’s just that infant memories are much more fragile.

Carolyn Rovee-Collier argues it is doubtful whether infantile amnesia really exists (Rovee-Collier, 1999).

It certainly appears our brains can lay down long-term memories even in the first year of life.

The reason it is unusual to retain infant memories from that time into adulthood is probably because of the limited capacity of our early memory systems and the intervening years during which we inevitably forget.

→ This article is part of a series on 10 crucial developmental psychology studies:

  1. When infant memory develops
  2. How self-concept emerges in infants
  3. How children learn new concepts
  4. The importance of attachment styles
  5. When infants learn to imitate others
  6. Theory of mind reveals the social world
  7. Understanding object permanence
  8. How infants learn their first word
  9. The six types of play
  10. Piaget’s stages of development theory

.

This Driving Mental Force Improves Your Memory (M)

The quality could be related to career, parenthood, activism, religion, family ties, artistic endeavours, or many other things.

The quality could be related to career, parenthood, activism, religion, family ties, artistic endeavours, or many other things.


Keep reading with a membership

• Adverts removed
• Cancel at any time
• 14 day money-back guarantee


Members can sign in below:

Why Taking A Break Boosts Memory And Learning (M)

The spacing effect in learning was discovered over 100 years ago, but scientist are only now beginning to understand it.

The spacing effect in learning was discovered over 100 years ago, but scientist are only now beginning to understand it.


Keep reading with a membership

• Adverts removed
• Cancel at any time
• 14 day money-back guarantee


Members can sign in below:

7 Sins of Memory: Schacter’s Guide To Memory Failures

Schacter’s ‘seven sins of memory’ are transience, absent-mindedness, blocking, misattribution, suggestibility, bias and persistence.

Schacter’s ‘seven sins of memory’ are transience, absent-mindedness, blocking, misattribution, suggestibility, bias and persistence.

“Memory itself is an internal rumour.” –George Santayana

The word rumour captures an aspect of memory perfectly.

When we delve backwards, moments never return in their original clarity; they return as rumours of the original event.

Faces have been switched, names deleted, words edited – sometimes it’s as though we weren’t even there.

Psychologists have found that right from the moment an event occurs, is laid down in memory (or not), to the moment we try to retrieve it (or can’t), our minds are fallible.

Harvard psychologist Professor Daniel L. Schacter has classified memory’s slips, ambiguities and downright lies into the ‘seven sins of memory’: transience, absent-mindedness, blocking, misattribution, suggestibility, bias and persistence (Schacter, 1999).

But despite these ‘sins’, we still get by.

Memory is what makes us who we are.

Practically it enables us to function in everyday life.

Without it we would be lost, like those with severe amnesia who can’t remember who they are or achieve even the simplest of tasks.

So how can memory’s fallibility be reconciled with its abilities?

The seven sins of memory

This series of posts explores these sins and in turn uncovers some bizarre stories as well as shedding light on everyday occurrences.

The surprise is that many sins of memory have a redeeming feature; sometimes the very sin itself is the flipside of one of memory’s saintly qualities, one we couldn’t do without.

Here are Schachter’s seven sins of memory:

  1. Short-Term Memory vs. Long-Term Memory: Definition And Examples
  2. Absent-Mindedness: 2 Factors That Cause Forgetfulness
  3. Tip-of-the-Tongue Phenomenon Or Lethologica
  4. Misattribution: How Memories are Distorted and Invented
  5. Suggestibility: How Memory Is Biased By Suggestions
  6. Commitment and Consistency Bias: How It Warps Memory
  7. Long-Term Memory: When Persistence Is A Curse

.