What We Don’t Know About Shopping, Reading, Watching TV and Judging People

When shopping, reading, watching TV and judging other people we have little access to what’s going on in our own minds.

TV Sketch

[Photo by zebtron]

Psychology studies that rely on deceiving participants have shown we often have little clue what’s going on in our own minds. But what about in everyday situations where trickery isn’t involved?

Here are four everyday situations – shopping, reading, watching TV and judging other people – and four experiments that show how little we know in each situation about what’s really going on in our minds (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977).

Shopping

For this study researchers set themselves up in a mall pretending to carry out a consumer survey on nightgowns and nylon stockings. Passersby were asked to evaluate what they were told were four different nightgowns and four different pairs of stockings. In fact, all four items were identical.

Quite by accident they discovered a positional effect for the identical goods: people seemed to prefer the item that was on the far right. In fact this effect was really obvious for the stockings. The right-most pair, although identical to the left-most was preferred by a factor of four to one.

When asked why they had chosen a particular item, no one mentioned its position. Even when experimenters suggested to people that the position might have an effect, most participants looked at best very confused and at worst utterly dismissive.

Result: these people didn’t have a clue why they preferred one identical pair of stockings over another.

Reading

In this study participants read a passage from the novel ‘Rabbit, Run’ by John Updike. The extract from the book involves an emotionally charged scene in which an alcoholic mother, while washing her baby in the bath, accidentally drowns and kills her.

Four conditions were used:

  1. The scene was presented in its entirety
  2. A part of the scene – a description of the baby’s messy crib – was deleted.
  3. A different part of the scene – a physical description of the baby – was deleted.
  4. Both (2) and (3)

Afterwards participants rated the emotional impact of the particular passage they had read. Comparing the ratings of the participants in the four conditions showed that there was barely any difference between emotional ratings (certainly nothing statistically significant). It seemed the emotional impact of the extract was unaffected by deleting either or both of these sections.

Then, if they were in condition 2, 3, or 4 participants were shown the parts that had been deleted and asked if it would have made any difference to the emotional impact of the extract if they had been included.

Here’s what they found:

  • Every participant in condition 4 thought the emotional impact would have been increased if the deleted parts had been included.
  • 86% of those in condition 2 thought the emotional impact would have been increased if the missing part was included.
  • Two-thirds of those in condition 3 thought the emotional impact would have been increased if the missing part was included.

Result: participants thought a particular small part of the scene would increase the overall emotional impact of an extract from a story. In fact it made no measurable difference at all.

Watching TV

This was similar to the previous study on reading. Except in this one, the experimental group were distracted from watching TV by both a noise outside and poor focus on the TV.

The results, while not as comprehensive as the reading study, still showed that more than half of the participants thought the noise had impaired their interest in the film or judgement of the people in it. In fact the ratings from the control group again showed that it hadn’t affected their judgement at all.

Result: participants thought distractions impaired their enjoyment of a TV program – in fact it made no difference.

Judging other people

In a study I reported recently on ‘the halo effect‘ student’s judgements of a lecturer’s accent, mannerisms and physical appearance were all affected by his apparent likeability. But, again, when interviewed afterwards, the participants were completely unaware of what had influenced their judgements.

In fact many thought it was his accent, physical appearance and mannerisms that had affected his likeability.

Hidden workings

Altogether these four studies show how little access we have to unconscious processes during everyday activities. The first and last, about shopping and judging others, both show how we often fail to spot effects that are really there. The second two, on reading and watching TV, show how we sometimes assume effects are there, when really they’re not.

This is just a sample of some of the studies reported by Nisbett and Wilson back in the late 70s. There have been many more similar results since. They all help to reinforce the fact that in real-life everyday situations we often have little or no access to our own thought processes.

We probably spend much of our lives making plausible but ultimately incorrect guesses about the inner workings of our minds.

Same ol’, same ol’, eh?

This post is part of a series on the hidden workings of our unconscious:

  1. The Hidden Workings of Our Minds
  2. Our Secret Attitude Changes
  3. Why Problem Solving Itself is a Puzzle, Even to Poincare and Picasso
  4. » What We Don’t Know About Shopping, Reading, Watching TV and Judging People
  5. When We Are Fools to Ourselves
  6. At the Heart of Attraction Lies Confusion: Choice Blindness

References

Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231-259.

Author: Jeremy Dean

Psychologist, Jeremy Dean, PhD is the founder and author of PsyBlog. He holds a doctorate in psychology from University College London and two other advanced degrees in psychology. He has been writing about scientific research on PsyBlog since 2004. He is also the author of the book "Making Habits, Breaking Habits" (Da Capo, 2013) and several ebooks.

Get free email updates

Join the free PsyBlog mailing list. No spam, ever.