Exercise 4 Hours After Learning Boosts Long-Term Memory

Exercise does more than just boost mood.

Exercise does more than just boost mood.

Long-term memory is boosted by exercise four hours after learning, a new study finds.

Exercising directly after learning, though, provides no boost to memory whatsoever.

The study split people into three groups, all of whom learnt a series of picture-location associations.

One carried out exercise just after learning, another four hours later and the final group did no exercise.

The exercise involved 35 minutes of interval training on an exercise bike.

Two days after learning they returned to the lab to see how much they could remember — and to have their brains scanned.

It was those who had exercised four hours after learning who displayed the best recall.

In addition, brain scans revealed that exercise lead to more precise representations of memories in the hippocampus.

The authors write:

“Our results suggest that appropriately timed physical exercise can improve long-term memory and highlight the potential of exercise as an intervention in educational and clinical settings.”

The scientists are not sure yet why exercise after learning boosts memory.

However, they write:

“Considering that the exercise intervention took place after learning, delayed exercise most likely affected memory retention through an impact on memory consolidation.

[…]

…previous research suggests that exercise triggers the release of BDNF, plasticity-related products (PRPs), noradrenaline, and dopamine, among other substances that promote neural plasticity.

Such factors are critical for the consolidation of synaptic potentiation, as proposed in the synaptic tagging and capture (STC) hypothesis, and are also important for later stages of memory consolidation.

The study was published in the journal Current Biology (van Dongen et al., 2016).

The Spice That Can Block Bad Memories

Spice already linked to a variety of health benefits.

Spice already linked to a variety of health benefits.

Turmeric — which is used in curries — can block fear memories after a traumatic experience, recent research finds.

Turmeric has already been linked to a variety of medicinal properties, including helping to treat stomach problems, arthritis and diabetes.

The study on rats has shown that the yellow-coloured, ginger root could have psychological benefits as well.

Professor Glenne Schafe, who led the research, said:

“We showed that rats freely fed a diet enriched with curcumin [found in turmeric] have impaired encoding of fear memories.

We also showed that rats with a pre-existing fear memory can lose that memory when it is recalled while they are eating a curcumin-enriched diet.”

Rats in the study who were fed the curcumin had their bad memories repressed for a long time.

Curcumin seemed to prevent the fearful memories being reconsolidated.

In other words: it was very hard for the fearful memories to return.

The authors write:

“Curcumin, a yellow-pigment compound found in the popular Indian spice turmeric (Curcuma longa), has been extensively investigated for its anti-inflammatory, chemopreventative and antidepressant properties.

Here, we examined the efficacy of dietary curcumin at impairing the consolidation and reconsolidation of a Pavlovian fear memory, a widely studied animal model of traumatic memory formation in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).”

Rats in the study, though, were given about 1 gram of curcumin per kilogram of their body weight.

This would mean giving 68 grams to an average human — an amount that is way to high.

Unfortunately the compound is very poorly absorbed by the body and turmeric only contains about 3% curcumin.

Future research will look at ways of increasing the concentration  and absorption of the compound.

The study’s authors conclude:

“…our findings indicate that a diet enriched with curcumin is capable of impairing fear memory consolidation and reconsolidation processes, findings that may have important clinical implications for the treatment of disorders such as PTSD that are characterized by unusually strong and persistently reactivated fear memories.”

The study was published in the journal Neuropsychopharmacology (Monsey et al., 2015).

Depression Linked To Pea-Sized Brain Structure That Backfires

An area of the brain that tracks negative events newly linked to depression.

An area of the brain that tracks negative events newly linked to depression.

A brain structure that tracks negative events backfires in depression, a new study finds.

The habenula — a structure the size of a pea — reacts strangely to negative events, researchers have found.

It could help explain a common symptom of depression: dwelling on negative memories.

Professor Jonathan Roiser, one of the study’s authors, said:

“A prominent theory has suggested that a hyperactive habenula drives symptoms in people with depression: we set out to test that hypothesis.

Surprisingly, we saw the exact opposite of what we predicted.

In people with depression, habenula activity actually decreased when they thought they would get a shock.

This shows that in depressed people the habenula reacts in a fundamentally different way.

Although we still don’t know how or why this happens, it’s clear that the theory needs a rethink.”

The results come from comparing brain scans (fMRI) of 25 people with depression with those of 25 people who never experienced depression.

Dr. Rebecca Lawson, the study’s first author, said:

“The habenula’s role in depression is clearly much more complex than previously thought.

From this experimental fMRI study we can draw conclusions about the effects of anticipated shocks on habenula activation in depressed individuals compared with healthy volunteers.

We can only speculate as to how this deactivation is linked to symptoms, but it could be that this ancient part of the brain actually plays a protective role against depression.

Animal experiments have shown that stimulating the habenula leads to avoidance, and it is possible that this occurs for mental as well as physical negative events.

So one possible explanation is that the habenula may help us to avoid dwelling on unpleasant thoughts or memories, and when this is disrupted you get the excessive negative focus that is common in depression.”

The study was published in the journal Molecular Psychiatry (Lawson et al., 2016).

Brain image from Shutterstock

How To Hack Your Memory To Follow-Through On Intentions

The simplest strategy for remembering to do something in the future does work.

The simplest strategy for remembering to do something in the future does work.

‘Reminders by association’ are a great tool to help you remember to do something in the future, new research demonstrates.

Here are a few examples of ‘reminders by association’:

  • A picture of your family by your desk reminds you to call them and tell them you will be late home.
  • A piece of litter on the floor reminds you to put the bins out.

While these might be accidental reminders, they can also be set up intentionally.

That’s what psychologists did in a study newly published in the journal Psychological Science.

Participants were given a variety of cues to remember to do something in the future.

One involved picking up a paper-clip, another was seeing an elephant statue, another was a poster of the alien from the Toy Story movies.

Little environmental cues like this were enough to double the number of people who remembered to perform some future action.

Dr Todd Rogers, the study’s first author, said

“Our results suggest that people are more likely to follow through on their good intentions if they are reminded to follow through by noticeable cues that appear at the exact place and time in which follow-through can occur.”

The beauty of reminders by association is that they don’t rely on any technology or other gizmos, which are likely to malfunction, run out of batteries or not remind you at exactly the right time.

They work so well because they are built into the environment around us.

The key to remembering to do something is getting the reminder at exactly the right time.

The study was published in the journal Psychological Science (Rogers & Milkman et al., 2016).

Image credit: annamo

Antibiotics Hurt Memory And Slow Brain Cell Growth

262 million prescriptions are written for antibiotics in one year in the US alone.

262 million prescriptions are written for antibiotics in one year in the US alone.

Antibiotics that are strong enough to kill the gut bacteria also stop new brain cells growing.

Scientists have found that brain cell growth in the hippocampus — a region vital for memory — is slowed by prolonged antibiotic use.

The effects can be countered, however, with exercise and probiotics.

Dr Susanne Asu Wolf, a senior study author, said:

“We found prolonged antibiotic treatment might impact brain function.

But probiotics and exercise can balance brain plasticity and should be considered as a real treatment option.”

In the research mice were given enough antibiotics to clear out their intestines of all microbes.

Their memories and brains were then compared with untreated mice.

The researchers found that mice who lost their healthy gut bacteria performed worse on the memory tests.

They also showed deficits in their ability to produce new brain cells.

The adverse effects, though, could be reversed.

Mice who were given probiotics and who exercised recovered both their memory and their ability to create new brain cells.

Dr Wolf said:

“The magnitude of the action of probiotics on Ly6Chi cells, neurogenesis, and cognition impressed me.”

Many studies are now showing the importance of ‘good’ intestinal bacteria for our mental health.

“Bacteria in the intestine can play an important role in causing anxiety and depression, new research concludes.

It helps explain recent research suggesting probiotics can stop sad moods getting worse.

Probiotics may work to help stabilise the bacteria in the gut.

Another recent study also found probiotics may reduce anxiety.”

And antibiotics have also been linked to mental confusion and even delirium in some patients.

The study was published in the journal Cell Reports (Möhle et al., 2016).

Running Like This Improves Memory In 15 Minutes

Working memory improved in just 15 minutes.

Working memory improved in just 15 minutes.

Running barefoot improves memory more than running with shoes on, a new study finds.

The benefits may come from the extra demands placed on the brain while barefoot running.

For example, you have to avoid stones and anything else that may damage your feet.

The type of memory tested in the study is called ‘working memory’.

The brain uses working memory to recall and process information.

Dr Tracy Alloway, the study’s first author, said:

“Working memory is increasingly recognized as a crucial cognitive skill, and these findings are great news for people looking for a fun way to boost their working memory.”

The study had 72 people running either barefoot of with shoes for about 15 minutes.

People ran at whatever pace was comfortable with them.

Memory tests afterwards showed that those running barefoot scored 16% better on working memory tests.

Dr Alloway continued:

“The little things often have the greatest impact.

This research shows us that we can realize our cognitive potential and enjoy ourselves at the same time.

If we take off our shoes and go for a run, we can finish smarter than when we started.”

The study was published in the journal Perceptual and Motor Skills (Alloway et al., 2016).

Barefoot image from Shutterstock

The Common Infection Linked To Mental Illness And Memory Problems

Men with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder were more likely to have a history of this infection.

Men with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder were more likely to have a history of this infection.

Candida yeast infections are linked to serious mental health problems, new research finds.

Men with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder were more likely to have a history of infection.

Women with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder who also had a Candida infection were more likely to have poorer memory.

Dr Emily Severance, the study’s first author, said:

“It’s far too early to single out Candida infection as a cause of mental illness or vice versa.

However, most Candida infections can be treated in their early stages, and clinicians should make it a point to look out for these infections in their patients with mental illness.”

Nevertheless, the study suggests there are critical links between psychiatric disorders, memory problems and yeast infections.

Candida infections can be tackled by washing properly, lowering sugar intake and avoiding unnecessary antibiotics.

Candida albicans is a fungus which naturally occurs in the human digestive tract.

It can cause rashes as well as sexually transmittable yeast infections.

Dr Severance said:

“Although we cannot demonstrate a direct link between Candida infection and physiological brain processes, our data show that some factor associated with Candida infection, and possibly the organism itself, plays a role in affecting the memory of women with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, and this is an avenue that needs to be further explored.

Because Candida is a natural component of the human body microbiome, yeast overgrowth or infection in the digestive tract, for example, may disrupt the gut-brain axis.

This disruption in conjunction with an abnormally functioning immune system could collectively disturb those brain processes that are important for memory.”

The study was published in the journal npj Schizophrenia (Severance et al., 2016).

Study: 4 Herbs That Influence Both Mood And Memory

Research finds the herbs that can improve mood, memory and induce calmness.

Research finds the herbs that can improve mood, memory and induce calmness.

Chamomile, peppermint, rosemary and lavender can all affect mood and memory, new research finds.

Peppermint tea can improve alertness while chamomile does indeed provide a calming effect.

Smelling rosemary, meanwhile, improved the memories of people over 65 by an average of 15%.

Lavender, though, impaired their memory.

The conclusions come from a series of studies which compared memory and thinking skills before and after exposure to various  herbs.

Dr Mark Moss, one of the study’s authors, said:

“Peppermint has a reputation for being psychologically or mentally alerting.

It picks you up and makes you feel a little bit brighter, so we endeavoured to test this out by giving people peppermint tea, or chamomile tea, which is a more calming drink and then put them through some computerised tests.

We found that those people who had drunk the peppermint tea had better long-term memory.

They were able to remember more words and pictures that they had seen.

Dr Moss continued:

“In contrast, the people who had the chamomile were slower in responding to tasks.

Rosemary meanwhile has a reputation about being associated with memory – even Shakespeare said ‘rosemary is for remembrance’ – and it’s also associated with being invigorating.

We have found that people are more alert after being in a room that has rosemary aroma in it.

We tested prospective memory – our ability to remember to remember to do something – on people over 65 years of age, to see if we could improve their ability and we found that rosemary could do that.

This is potentially very important because prospective memory, for example, enables you to remember to take your medication at certain times of the day.

Dr Moss said the varied results for different herbs were interesting:

“It is interesting to see the contrasting effects that different herbs can have on both mood and memory, and our research suggests that that they could have beneficial effects, particularly in older age groups.

If you were otherwise healthy then this research suggests that there is an opportunity to have an improved memory.”

The findings were presented at the annual British Psychological Society Conference in Nottingham (26-28 April 2016).

You Can Remember More Words With This Imagery Strategy

Study tests effects of mental imagery on memory.

Study tests effects of mental imagery on memory.

Mental imagery is an effective way to improve memory and reduce some false memories, new research finds.

People in the study who were told to create images for words were better able to remember them.

Images also helped people avoid false memories.

Ms Merrin Oliver, the study’s first author, said:

“Creating images improved participants’ memories and helped them commit fewer errors, regardless of what kind of list we gave them.”

False memories can be related to misremembering the source, Ms Oliver explained:

“We aren’t good at judging the source of our memories.

These lists usually remind people of a word that they didn’t actually study, so they mistakenly recollect studying words similar to those on the list.”

For the research, one group were told to use imagery to remember a list of words while the other were just told to remember the words.

Ms Oliver said:

“Our study suggests more detailed imagery instructions are necessary to help filter out false memories during a recognition test, where false memories are typically very high.

People should create detailed images with unique characteristics to help avoid the endorsement of false memories on recognition-based tests like true/false or multiple-choice assessments, where you are tempted by lures and possible false memories.”

The study was published in The Journal of General Psychology (Oliver et al., 2016).

How Memory Works: 21 Psychological Insights

Brain image from Shutterstock

How To Write Down Notes You Will Remember

Study compares writing by hand with real and virtual keyboards.

Study compares writing by hand with real and virtual keyboards.

Writing by hand strengthens memory in comparison to writing on a real or virtual keyboard, new research finds.

The motor feedback from the process of writing along with the sense of touching paper and pen helps people learn.

Areas of the brain vital to language are more strongly activated by the physical activity.

The study compared 36 people writing in three different ways:

  • on a conventional keyboard,
  • on a virtual keyboard (like an iPad),
  • or handwriting.

They were read a list of words which they wrote down.

Their memory for the words was then tested for both free recall and recognition.

The results showed that the method of writing down the words did not effect recognition, but free recall was better for handwriting.

Since it is much harder to recall a word than to recognise it, memory was stronger for handwritten words.

The authors explain the study’s results:

“…keyboard writing per se (whether on a virtual or a conventional keyboard) attenuates or disrupts memory for what is written.

However, with respect to aspects of word recall, our findings indicate that there may be certain cognitive benefits to handwriting which may not be fully retained in keyboard writing.”

The advantage of handwriting is that the attention is all on the same thing at the same time.

The study’s authors think the problem with keyboard writing could be partly down to a split in attention:

“The visual attention of keyboard writers is split between looking at the emerging text and looking at the keyboard on which they write.

From a visual-spatial perspective, a keyboard separates the “motor area” (or input area) where the letters are being produced (the keyboard) from the visual presentation area of the letters (the screen; or output area).

A keyboard thereby provides less real-time sensory and visual information about the writer’s own writing process, a fact which may result in less robust mental representations of the words.

One consequence of such a separation may be that the writer engages less with the written text and consequently is provided with an attenuated visual memory of the word, than in the handwriting condition, where the subject may fixate near the point where the physical writing takes place.”

The study was published in the Journal of Writing Research (Mangen et al., 2016).

Handwriting image from Shutterstock

Get free email updates

Join the free PsyBlog mailing list. No spam, ever.